Category Deficits and Paradoxical Dissociations in
Alzheimer’s Disease and Herpes Simplex Encephalitis
& Most studies examining category specificity are single-case
patient groups were compared with age- and education-
studies of patients with living or nonliving deficits. Never-
matched healthy controls. The profile in each patient was
theless, no explicit or agreed criteria exist for establishing
examined for consistency across tasks and across different
category-specific deficits in single cases regarding the type of
analyses; however, both proved to be inconsistent. One
analyses, whether to compare with healthy controls, the
striking finding was the presence of paradoxical dissocia-
number of tasks, or the type of tasks. We examined two
tions (i.e., patients who were impaired for living things on
groups of patients with neurological pathology frequently
one task and nonliving things on another task). The findings
accompanied by impaired semantic memory (19 patients with
have significant implications for how we determine cate-
Alzheimer’s disease and 15 with Herpes Simplex Encephalitis).
gory effects and, more generally, for the methods used to
Category knowledge was examined using three tasks (picture
document double dissociations across individual cases in this
naming, naming-to-description, and feature verification). Both
nonliving items that are named (verified, identified, and
Although various tasks and statistical techniques have
so on) rather than comparing the patient data with
been used to try to document category effects, a critical
control data. This may be misleading about the presence
and surprisingly unaddressed question concerns what
and even direction of category effects. Laws (in press)
would be considered acceptable as evidence of a
noted several consequences of failing to evaluate patient
‘‘category effect’’ (Laws, 2004, in press). Most category
naming against the performance of matched normal
effects reflect an initial demonstration of poor picture
healthy controls. The most common analysis in over
naming for one category and, in some cases, poor
two-thirds of all category-specific studies is the within-
picture naming is the only evidence presented (e.g., in
patient comparison of the absolute scores (using x2:
studies of Alzheimer’s patients, see Laws, in press). One
Laws, in press); and this reveals quite different dissoci-
thing is certain, that is, that picture naming is implicitly
ations from those disclosed when a control comparison
viewed as the main task for confirming the presence of a
is included. Indeed, Laws, Gale, et al. (2005) have
category effect (and then that the additional tasks are
demonstrated how patients may show false negatives,
used to elaborate on that). This assumption reflects the
false positives, and even paradoxical dissociations (i.e.,
fact that most models predict that naming is impaired if
impaired for living on one task and for nonliving on
semantic memory is impaired (and vice-versa). Never-
another task). Hence, the absolute living–nonliving
theless, evidence has existed for some time to show that
naming difference alone (be it exceptionally large or
this is not necessarily the case. For example, Laws,
nonexistent) provides an unreliable indicator for both
Evans, Hodges, and McCarthy (1995) described patient
the presence and the direction of category effects.
SE who had impaired (associative, nonvisual) semantics
This raises an important related methodological issue
for animals but could name pictures of animals.
concerning how we define a category effects through
Recent studies have raised issues about the statistical
a test profile. In fact, no a priori test profile exists
methods used to assess category effects with naming
for documenting a category disorder (e.g., a category
data (Laws, in press; Laws, Gale, Leeson, & Crawford,
difference for picture naming, attribute verification,
2005). In particular, the common practice of making
naming-to-description, fluency, drawing or any com-
comparisons between the absolute number of living and
bination of these). It also raises a critical issue con-cerning whether such disorders are empirically and/ortheoretically driven. For example, we could make an
1Nottingham Trent University, UK, 2University of Padua, Italy
arbitrary empirical decision such that for a category
*Now at the University of Hertfordshire, UK
effect to be convincing, minimally requires a patient
D 2005 Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17:9, pp. 1453–1459
to show the same significant category effect on three
matched controls. Two of the Alzheimer’s patients were
tasks: A (picture naming), B (drawing), and C (attribute
impaired on all three category tasks (see Table 1). Nine
verification). Nevertheless, what if another patient is
were impaired on picture naming (7 living and 2 non-
impaired on A and B, but not C? This patient may still
living), 10 on feature verification (all living), and 3 on
have a category disorder and tell us something poten-
tially more important about the true nature of category
Critically, the analyses revealed category inconsisten-
effects (e.g., that they might be related to visual knowl-
cies across tasks. One patient showed a paradoxical
dissociation: patient LZ had a differential deficit for living
Finally, although the vast majority of category-specific
things on the feature verification task, but for nonliving
studies use a case study approach (for a review, see
Laws, in press), a minority have used between-groupcomparisons. One reason that group studies have been
less popular in this area is because group analyses maycancel out individual category effects. For example, if
The comparison with results from x2 points to a number
some patients showed a living deficit and some a
of both false positives (7) and false negatives (5). In one
nonliving deficit, then the overall pattern might simply
patient (SL), x2 produced significant dissociations on all
reveal poor performance on both (see Gonnerman,
three tasks, however, when referenced to control data,
Anderson, Devlin, Kempler, & Seidenberg, 1997). Thisis especially likely with severely neurologically impairedcases. The following experiments compare category
Table 1. Category Performance in Alzheimer’s Patients
knowledge on three tasks in healthy controls with
patients with Alzheimer’s disease and Herpes SimplexEncephalitis (HSE); and are designed to explore how
category effects might be determined.
The incidence and pattern of category specificity across
Alzheimer’s patients as a group (Tippett, Grossman, &Farah 1996; Silveri, Daniele, Guistolisi, & Gainotti 1991)
and for individual Alzheimer’s patients (Laws, Leeson, &
Gale, 2003; Garrard, Patterson, Watson, & Hodges, 1998;
Gonnerman et al., 1997; Mauri, Daum, Satori, Riesch, &Birbaumer, 1994) have been inconsistent.
Most analyses of Alzheimer’s patients have reported
living deficits, a minority has reported nonliving deficits,
some report both, and still others find no categoryspecificity in Alzheimer’s patients (for a review, see
Laws, Gale, et al., 2005). Some of this variability may
stem from previously ignored methodological problems. In particular, Laws, Gale, et al. (2005) note that controls
in these studies are often performing at ceiling level, and
have shown how this may distort the incidence and even
direction of category effects. An additional issue con-cerns the observation that almost all of the studies
examining category effects in Alzheimer’s patients have
relied exclusively on picture naming as the test ofcategory. The current study addresses these points by
testing picture naming, naming-to-description, and fea-
ture verification in Alzheimer’s patients and controls.
The criterion for a living–nonliving dissociation was that
*L = differential living deficit; NL = differential nonliving deficit; x2 =patient impaired using x2; s = strong dissociation; c = classical
a patient must show a discrepancy that would be
estimated to occur in <1% of the population of healthy
+ = living advantage; À = nonliving advantage.
none emerged as significant. Using the criterion of
on two of the three tasks. Two HSE patients (DD
category-consistent dissociations across all three tasks,
and MF) also displayed paradoxical dissociations (see
two patients (MS and AE) showed living impairments.
A further six patients were impaired on two tasks;
critically, in patient LZ, the deficits were paradoxical,that is, impaired for living on one task (feature verifica-
tion) and nonliving (picture naming) on another task. The case of LZ is of particular importance. Given that
Again, a large number of false negatives occur when
the features used in the features verification task were
using x2 (11 for HSE patients). As with Alzheimer’s
all perceptual/visual, and given that semantic activation
patients, an absolute (nonreferenced) comparison is
from pictures is triggered from visual features of the
likely to underestimate rather than overestimate the
picture itself, this paradoxical dissociation is particularly
incidence of category dissociations. Inconsistency across
surprising. The lack of consistency across analyses and
tasks for an individual patient again highlights the fact
the finding of a paradoxical dissociation across tasks
that performance on any one task cannot provide a
emphasize why performance on any one task cannot
reliable marker for a category effect—this includes
provide a reliable indicator of the direction of a category
feature verification (which was the most sensitive and
reliable task in both groups for detecting significantcategory differences).
Furthermore, the analysis of HSE patients accentuates
how absolute scores can be misleading. Consider BM,
who showed a significant differential deficit for living
things on feature verification in the x2 analysis (76% vs. 90%), but not when compared to controls (although the
In contrast to the studies of category effects in groups ofAlzheimer’s patients, many of the reported case studiesof patients with category-specific disorders are people
Table 2. Category Performance in HSE Patients (% Living and
who have suffered from HSE (22/47 living cases, and not
one nonliving case; see anatomical review by Gainotti,2000). The original report of category effects was in a
series of four HSE patients (Warrington & Shallice,
1984). As Gainotti (2000) argues, when referring to
HSE, that ‘‘the selective impairment of Living things,
far from being an occasional phenomenon resultingfrom idiosyncratic factors, is strongly associated with a
Following the initial reports of Warrington and
Shallice (1984), other patients suffering from the same
pathology who also displayed living thing deficits in-
creased confidence in the claim that ‘‘real’’ category
specificity can be found predominantly in this group ofpatients. Capitani, Laiacona, Mahon, and Caramazza
(2003) reported, in their database, 26/61 patients with
HSE in the group impaired for living and 1/18 in that
impaired for nonliving. Nevertheless, these figures referto the incidence of HSE category-specific patients in the
total population of category-specific patients. No data
are available regarding the incidence of category speci-ficity among the HSE population. For this reason, the
second part of this study examines the incidence and
type of category effects that occur in a series of patients
The data were analyzed using the same methods out-
*L = differential living deficit; NL = differential nonliving deficit; x2 =patient impaired using x2; s = strong dissociation; c = classical
lined in Experiment 1. One HSE patient was impaired
across all three tasks (SR). Five patients were impaired
+ = living advantage; À = nonliving advantage.
difference was larger than 6 of 8 patients who showed a
conversely, does the failure to find a large absolute
significant difference using the Revised Standardized
difference across category refute the presence of a cat-
Difference Test [RSDT]). The absolute size of the differ-
egory deficit. Indeed, several HSE and Alzheimer’s pa-
ence is therefore not a definitive marker for the pres-
tients showed large absolute differences that were sig-
ence or absence of a differential deficit. Moreover,
nificant using within-patient x2 analyses, but not when
consider patients GE (90 vs. 86%) and ZG (90 vs.
referenced to control data (e.g., HSE patients such as
82%), who showed better absolute feature verification
BM, DD, and SG for picture naming; and Alzheimer’s
for living than for nonliving things, yet had differential
patients such as RN, MR, and VP for naming-to-
living deficits (indeed, classical dissociations). So, not
description). Conversely and more commonly, x2 analy-
only do absolute differences provide no guarantee to the
ses led to false negatives in both HSE (11 dissociations)
presence of a differential deficit, they provide no guar-
and Alzheimer’s (7 dissociations) patients. This reflects
antee about the direction of any deficit.
the simple fact that absolute differences are critical fordemonstrating deficits when using x2; however, healthycontrols may show category advantages, and therefore,even a lack of absolute difference in patients may be
important. For example, analysis of picture-naming for
This study investigated the validity of the criteria that are
HSV patients DD and SG indicated no category differ-
commonly used to document the presence of category-
ence (56 vs. 57% and 76 vs. 78%), yet both showed sig-
specific deficits, in particular, the strong dependence
nificant dissociations when referenced to control data.
upon within-patient comparisons of absolute living and
Conversely, Alzheimer’s patient FR showed a 31% dif-
nonliving scores rather than comparing patient perform-
ference in living and nonliving naming-to-description
ance with that of healthy controls. Additionally, most
(19 vs. 50%), but this was not significant when referenced
studies have relied upon establishing category disorders
on picture-naming tasks; and in some studies (especiallythose examining Alzheimer’s patients), rely solely upon
picture naming as evidence of category specificity (Laws,
Gale, et al., 2005). The current study illustrates how suchmethods lead to erroneous conclusions about both the
As outlined in the Introduction, the extant literature
presence and even the type of category deficit exhib-
relies heavily upon using impaired picture naming to
ited by patients. These findings have implications for a
identify category effects; other tests may be used subse-
range of questions relating to category disorders and
quently to elaborate on this, but picture naming has
raise methodological and theoretical issues regarding
assumed a role as the sine qua non test of category
what might be regarded as acceptable evidence for
specificity. Therefore, the failure to find a category
dissociations and double dissociations—especially when
difference on naming tasks is typically viewed as under-
used to make claims about the fractionation of cogni-
mining the presence of any category effect (Laws, 1998;
Although most cognitive psychologists are comfort-
able with the notion of impaired naming and intact
semantics (i.e., lexical deficits), the converse—impaired
The current study highlights the unreliability of using
semantics and intact naming—have been less frequently
within-patient comparison of absolute scores. As already
reported, perhaps because of the assumption that it
mentioned, studies of Alzheimer’s patients frequently
ought not to happen within most currently accepted
assess picture naming only, and in line with the data
models (Laws, Evans, et al., 1995). Nonetheless, the
presented here, would be likely to misrepresent not
current study documents several Alzheimer’s (2/15:
only the incidence, but even the direction of a category
13%)1 and HSE (3/15: 20%) patients who showed a
effect. The nature of x2 analyses means, of course, that
category effect in semantics (using feature verification),
studies always assume the deficit to be reflected in the
but no category effect on either of two naming tasks.
smaller of any two scores obtained. The current study
Some patients (2/19 Alzheimer’s and 4/15 HSE )
shows how this metric is unreliable and confirms our
showed category deficits on tasks other than picture
recent findings in two other groups of Alzheimer’s pa-
naming, including patients (e.g., Alzheimer’s patient FB
and 2 HSE patients DD and TL) with impaired perform-
The widespread use of within-patient comparisons to
ance on all tasks except picture naming. Therefore,
determine category deficits may well have distorted the
using picture naming as a critical test of category effects
incidence, and therefore, the ratio of living to nonliving
would miss some patients who showed consistent def-
deficits reported in the literature. Even if patients show a
icits on other tasks. Indeed, reliance on any one task
patently large absolute difference across category, this
may lead to a quite different conclusion from that if
does not necessarily denote a category disorder; or
another task had been chosen. Moreover, feature veri-
fication proved to be a much more sensitive test for
We must also consider the possibility that paradoxical
detecting category effects (albeit all living deficits) than
double dissociations reflect confounding variables. It
picture naming or naming-to-description (although
might be argued, for example, that f luctuations in
there were also differences across pathologies). This
attention could impact differentially over the test session
suggests that the direction of category effects some-
and potentially affect one category more than the other.
times seems to depend upon which test is chosen as the
This is unlikely because it would require that the
reference test (a prospect that has not been previously
confound interacts highly selectively with category. Liv-
entertained). In this context, it is worth noting that pa-
ing and nonliving stimuli (on all tests) were randomly
tients who show living disorders tend to be agnosic, and
intermixed when presented, so a factor such as attention
therefore, tested with picture naming; however, sev-
fluctuation would have to impact only when items from
eral nonliving cases have been aphasic, and thus, were
one of the two categories were presented. This seems
not tested with picture naming, but with tasks such
even more implausible in cases when we consider
as picture–name matching (see Laws, 2004). Hence, it
paradoxical dissociations (i.e., in the opposite direction
is common for different category effects to rely upon
on a second test). Consider the case of the HSE patient
different testing procedures; and as such, again the
MF (see Figure 1), who showed a classical double
existing literature may be prone to some of the issues
dissociation across tasks. His picture naming was below
the 1% for living things (and normal for nonlivingthings); and below the 1% for nonliving on featureverification (but normal for living things). In this con-
Dissociations and Paradoxical Dissociations
text, we would argue that the dissociations are robust to
Dissociations often form the basis for speculations about
cognitive architecture and modularity especially when
Another potential confound concerns the possibility
they are doubly dissociated between patients. The cur-
that the dissociations reported here (whether consistent
rent study shows, however, that dissociations can occur
or paradoxical) are chance findings emerging from quite
within a patient. Within-patient double dissociations
noisy patient data, and that multiple analyses might
across tasks (e.g., a living on Task A and a nonliving
increase the likelihood of spurious outcomes. Indeed,
on Task B) that are believed to have some critical
typical statistical/methodological approaches may well
processing stage in common, raise questions about the
be prone to producing spurious and chance findings in
double-dissociation methodology in single-case studies
case study analyses. Nevertheless, Monte Carlo simula-
and the interpretation of category effects per se. At atheoretical level, many models assume that deficits insemantics will have ‘‘knock-on’’ effects for naming; andso, such models have difficulty accounting for paradox-ical dissociations at the level of semantics and naming.
Paradoxical double dissociations pose problems for
double dissociations at a variety of levels of compari-son including: across tasks (as described here), withintasks, and patients (Laws, Gale, et al., 2005); and ofcourse, across patients and across tasks (the typicalapproach in category specificity and cognitive neuro-psychology more generally). Given that paradoxicaldissociations arise, how might we distinguish a paradox-ical dissociation from a real double dissociation (i.e.,one that might be used to ground theories of cognitionor ‘‘carve cognition at its modular joints’’)?
How should paradoxical dissociations be interpreted?
Of course, it might be argued that paradoxical dissocia-tions are simply unreliable. Indeed, because we did notretest patients, we have no way of confirming whetherparadoxical dissociations are reliable. Nonetheless, thereliability of paradoxical dissociations has to be viewed
Figure 1. A classical paradoxical double dissociation between living
alongside the fact that reliability is hardly ever examined
and nonliving things within one HSE patient (MF ). Note: Patient MF
for dissociations in single-case studies. Indeed, follow-up
displays a classical double dissociation across category (i.e., impaired
analyses of the same patient by same or other research
picture naming for living things, but normal nonliving thing naming). On feature verification, he shows normal living and impaired nonliving
groups are rare and sometimes contradictory (Laws,
performance. Classical double dissociations (often with weaker
1998). Hence, it is crucial for future studies to examine
evidence than here) typically provide the strongest evidence for the
the reliability of all dissociations.
separation of cognitive processed (or architecture).
tions show that the RSDT (Crawford & Garthwaite,
2002). Half of the stimuli required a ‘‘yes’’ response and
2005) used in the current study provides excellent
half required a ‘‘no’’ response.
control over Type 1 error rate (even when patient data
3. Naming-to-description: Originally published by
are heavily skewed through poor performance).
Lambon-Ralph, Howard, Nightingale, and Ellis (1998)
The dissociations reported here within one patient
as a feature verification task, it was adapted to Italian as a
are indistinguishable from those typically reported be-
naming-to-description task. Sixteen animals and 12 ob-
tween patients in the category-specific literature. De-
jects were used. Each concept was described by a per-
spite the equivalence of paradoxical and ‘‘regular’’
ceptual and a functional/associative description, thus
dissociations (and double dissociations) in terms of their
yielding a total of 32 descriptions for living and 24
acceptability as empirical evidence, it may be that both
are unreliable. Hence, we should not take any one (oreven some combinations) test as prima facie evidence ofa category effect in one direction. As noted above,
reliability, both across task and across time, are criticalissues for future studies to consider in the case study
This experiment included 15 (7 women and 8 men)patients with HSE and 12 healthy control subjects (6women and 6 men). Herpes Simplex patients were all
recruited in three hospitals located in Veneto (Italy),
were native speakers of Italian, and all satisfied thefollowing criteria: all had polymerase-chain-reaction-
proven HSE; all showed pathologic changes at CT scan
The study included 19 (15 women and 4 men) patients
or MRI which were usually bilateral, in the medial-
with probable Alzheimer’s dementia and 15 healthy
temporal and inferior frontal areas; all underwent treat-
elderly control subjects (9 women and 6 men). The 19
ment with Acyclovir; and all were examined between 13
Alzheimer’s patients met the National Institute of Neu-
rological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alz-
The HSE patients and controls were matched for
heimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association
mean (SD) age [53.66 (14.77) vs. 54.47 (12.63): F(1,25) =
(NINCDS/ADRDA) criteria for probable Alzheimer’s dis-
0.22, p = .88] and mean (SD) years of education [7.73
ease (McKhann et al., 1984). All 19 patients had
(3.17) vs. 10.41 (4.31): F(1,25) = 3.47, p > .05]. They did
Hachinski scores of less than 4 (Hachinski et al.,
differ significantly in their mean [SD] MMSE scores
1975) and a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)
[24.66 (3.26) vs. 28.68 (0.72): F(1,25) = 17.31, p < .001].
score below 24/30 (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh,1975). All underwent CT or MRI scanning, together
with a screening battery, to exclude treatable causes ofdementia. Patients with major depression, a history of
The same three tasks used for the Alzheimer’s patient
stroke or TIA, alcoholism, head injury, or major med-
were administered to the HSE patients.
ical illnesses were excluded. Patients were recruited inthree hospitals and in four nursing homes located in
Veneto (Italy) and were an unselected series.
The Alzheimer’s patients and controls were matched
for age [mean (SD): 77.9 (8.7) vs. 75.33 (4.2): F(1,33) =
The data for each individual participant were examined
1.02, p > .05] and years of education [4.73 (0.80) vs. 6.28
by comparing performance with their respective control
(4.76): F(1, 33) = 1.5, p > .05]. They did, of course,
group using the RSDT (Crawford & Garthwaite, 2005)
differ significantly in their MMSE [19.31 (2.45) vs. 27.4
for testing for deficits and dissociations in single-case
(1.11): F(1,33) = 126, p < .001].
studies. Of course, it is possible for patients to be im-paired at naming living or nonliving things, but that thedifference between their scores does not reach signifi-
cance; equally, a patient may be severely impaired on
1. Picture naming: 32 living (20 animals and 12 fruits
both tasks, but still show differential impairment. This
and vegetables) and 32 nonliving items matched for
method examines whether the discrepancy observed for
frequency, familiarity, and visual complexity (Sartori,
the patient is significantly different from the discrepan-
cies observed for controls and provides a point estimate
2. Feature verification: 80 living and 64 nonliving
of the abnormality of the individual’s discrepancy (i.e., it
items (e.g., ‘‘a tiger has stripes’’). All features were
estimates the percentage of the population that would
perceptual. The examinee is required to respond if the
obtain a more extreme discrepancy). Patient perform-
feature belongs to the concept or not (Sartori et al.,
ance was classified as impaired if the discrepancy score
was estimated to occur in less than 1% of the healthy
cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
Gainotti, G. (2000). What the locus of brain lesion tells
Patients were classified as displaying strong dissocia-
us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying
tions if they were (a) impaired at both tasks and (b)
showed a significant discrepancy across category; and
classified as displaying a classical dissociation if they
Garrard, P., Patterson, K., Watson, P. C., & Hodges, J. R.
were (a) impaired in one category, but performed
(1998). Category specific semantic loss in dementia ofAlzheimer’s type. Functional–anatomical correlations from
normally on the other and (b) showed a significant
discrepancy between the two categories (see Crawford
Gonnerman, L. M., Anderson, E. S., Devlin, J. T., Kempler, D.,
& Garthwaite, 2005; Crawford, Garthwaite, & Gray, 2003).
& Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). Double dissociation of
These methods of testing for deficits and for differ-
semantic categories in Alzheimer’s disease.
ences (i.e., dissociations) are to be preferred over the
Hachinski, V. C., Iliff, L. D., Zihka, E., Duboulay, G. H.,
use of z and zD as they treat the statistics of the control
McAllister, V. L., Marshall, J., Russell, R. W. R., & Symon, L.
sample as statistics rather than as population parame-
(1975). Cerebral blood flow in dementia.
ters. Moreover, Monte Carlo simulations show that the
RSDT (Crawford & Garthwaite, 2005) provides excellent
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Howard, D., Nightingale, G., &
control over Type 1 error rate (regardless of whether the
Ellis, A. W. (1998). Are living and non-livingcategory-specific deficits causally linked to impaired
data are heavily skewed, the control sample is small in
perceptual or associative knowledge? Evidence from a
size, or the correlation between tasks). We contrasted
results from application of this method with those
derived from applying, on the same data, those derived
Laws, K. R. (in press). ‘‘Illusions of Normality’’: A
from using x2—the method most frequently used to
methodological review of category-specific naming. Cortex.
evaluate discrepancies in living/nonliving accuracy in
Laws, K. R. (1998). A leopard never changes its spots.
single cases (Laws, in press). Programs to run the RSDT
Laws, K. R. (2004). Strange and neglected domains in
category specificity: The ‘‘normal’’ and the‘‘nonliving’’. History and Philosophy of Psychology, 6,19–39.
Laws, K. R., Evans, J. J., Hodges, J. R., & McCarthy, R. A. (1995).
Naming without knowing and appearance without
We thank Dr. Ivry and the reviewers for insights and helpful
associations—Evidence for constructive processes in
comments on an earlier version of this article.
Reprint requests should be sent to Dr. Keith R. Laws, School of
Laws, K. R., Gale, T. M., Leeson, V. C., & Crawford, J. R.
Psychology, University of Hertfordshire, College Lane, Hat-
(2005). When is category specific in dementia of Alzheimer’s
field, Hertfordshire AL10 9AB, UK, or via e-mail: keith.laws@
Laws, K. R, Leeson, V. C., & Gale, T. M. (2003). Inflated and
contradictory category naming deficits in Alzheimer’sdisease?
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R.,
Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of
Four dementia of Alzheimer’s type patients were not test-
Alzheimer disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work
Mauri, A., Daum, I., Sartori, G., Riesch, G., & Birbaumer, N.
(1994). Category-specific semantic impairment in
Alzheimer’s disease and temporal lobe dysfunction: Acomparative study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003).
What are the facts of semantic category-specific deficits? A
Sartori, G., Job, R., & Zago, S. (2002). A case of domain-specific
critical review of the clinical evidence.
semantic deficits. In E. M. E. Forde & G. W. Humphreys
(Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 25–49).
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2005). Testing for
suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case
Silveri, M. C., Daniele, A., Giustolisi, L., & Gainotti, G. (1991).
studies in neuropsychology: Evaluation of alternatives using
Dissociation between knowledge of living and non-living
Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations.
things in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.
Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Gray, C. D. (2003).
Tippett, L. J., Grossman, M., & Farah, M. J. (1996). The
Wanted: Fully operational definitions of dissociations in
semantic memory impairment of Alzheimer’s disease:
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975).
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific
‘‘Mini-mental state’’: A practical method for grading the
Learning from a Suicide This is an actual case history. For various reasons, despite the client having died, all names and possibleidentifiers have been changed. Details have been compiled from various sources. I was employed for a while as a psychological therapist in a Clinical Psychology department thatserved a rural area of Scotland, once a thriving mining community, now a little like th
Prescription claims (Jan 01, 2007 - Dec 31, 2007) The claims below include only those prescriptions that have been processed to date. 2007 Prescription claims Patient: Ingrid Richmond Plan paid Rx Number: 0393037 Date of service: 01/03/07 Drug information: Unithroid Tabs 100mcg Filled at: CVS PHARMACY #4401 Rx Number: 0401272 Date of service: 01/08/07 Drug informat